Homo sapiens are thought of as a unique species on earth. Several of the fundamental aspects are very
similar for humans and other species, such as the ability to have behavior conditioned
and several behavior motivating factors. In other areas, however, humans are
unique to other species.
Conditioning
Another behavior aspect
that humans and animals have in common is the ability to be easily conditioned.
As demonstrated by psychologist Ivan Pavlov and his dogs, animals can be
classically conditioned to various stimuli. This is very similar to the way
that humans, after being burned by hot shower water, can be classically
conditioned to quickly maneuver away from shower needles after a nearby toilet
is flushed. Operant conditioning, or making a certain behavior more or less
probable using punishment or reinforcement, has been observed for both humans
and animals. An individual who teaches a new trick to their dog by reinforcing
the desired behavior with treats is engaging in operant conditioning.
Similarly, the 4th grade teacher who gives out gold stars to
students who score A’s is using operant conditioning to raise student grades.
Hierarchy of Needs
Human behavior and
that of other animals are similar in many ways.
Psychologist Abraham Maslow attempted to describe animal and human
behavior in a paper entitled, A Theory of
Human Motvation. Maslow describes how in both humans and animals, certain
needs and drives have precedence over others, depending on recently they have
been satisfied as well as importance to well being. His ordering of the needs
in terms of relative precedence is now called Maslow’s hierarchy of needs.
Of the drives and
needs described by Maslow, physiological drives precede all others. Both humans
and animals experience what is called homeostasis, where physiological drives
such as air, food, or water provoke behavior that will satisfy the need, thus
temporarily allowing the individual to focus its attention elsewhere. Until the
physiological drive is reduced, however, it will preoccupy the individual. If
there is prolonged deprivation, the individual will experience pain. This
serves as evidence that physiological drives can be an exclusive behavior
organizer for both humans and animals. One distinction worth noting, however, is that
delayed gratification plays an important role in influencing human behavior.
Because of this, humans are able to escape pure physiological behavior
domination to achieve more socially oriented goals – something animals do not
generally engage in.
Following
physiological drives in Maslow’s hierarchy is safety needs. Just like
physiological drives, safety needs can be an exclusive behavior organizer for
humans and animals. While this can be
easily observed watching certain animal behavior, safety need-reducing behavior
by humans is less commonly observed in society. Cases where this concept is
illustrated include emergency situations, individuals with neurosis (excessive
and irrational anxiety), and in infants (observed when an infant starts crying
due to a perceived threat). For both humans and animals, an absence of
safety-need fulfillment is felt as fear. An important factor, exclusively for
humans in this case, is the role of culture. Because premature human deaths are
seen as sub-optimal for society, human culture aims to collectively reduce
societal safety needs. The impact of such is an enormous reduction of the
possibility that safety needs are left unsatisfied. Some even argue that this desire to reduce
societal safety needs is the driving force behind such fields as science,
philosophy, and religion that aim to increase prosperity.
Once
physiological and safety needs have been fulfilled, one’s attention may be
refocused to the next highest need in the hierarchy – love and belongingness
needs. For both humans and animals, negative emotions are generally experienced
during isolation. John Burkitt this calls a “reaction to potential social
contact.” He goes on to say that tigers have been observed playing in neutral
territorial zones and engaging in other activities that suggest social
stimulation. Just as humans do in the absence of friends, love, or children,
isolated tigers display apparent grief symptoms, such as loss of appetite.
After
love and belongingness needs in the hierarchy is esteem needs. Some might argue
that humans and not animals exclusively experience esteem needs. Burkitt argues
that esteem needs, though less obvious, may be observed in tiger behavior. He
says that degree of influence in one’s environment is a direct indicator of
one’s esteem, since tigers self-sort according to dominant/submissive
relationships. They then respond to challenges according to confidence level.
For both humans and animals, fulfillment of esteem needs results in feelings of
self-worth and confidence and failure to meet these needs results in
discouragement or in some cases neurosis.
Even
deeper in the hierarchy lies the need for self-actualization, or reaching one’s
full potential. Even though there may
be a few exceptions and exclusive factors, the first four needs described by
Maslow are experienced by humans and animals. This drive, on the other
hand, is one exclusively experienced by humans. Assuming that more urgent
drives have been satisfied, an individual will develop a need to be all that he
can be. In A Theory of Human Motivation, Maslow
describes this concept with the following phrase, “What a man can be, he must be,”(Maslow 383).
In conclusion, whether
humans are fundamentally different from other species is dependent on which
fundamental aspect being examined. Those aspects most important to survival of
a species, such as physiological drives and safety needs are generally more
similar between humans and animals. The aspects more relevant to cognition and
other unique human capabilities, such as the need for self-actualization,
generally have a larger distinction between humans and other species.
No comments:
Post a Comment